XUAN ZHAO
  • Home
  • Research
  • Flourish
  • Teaching
  • Resources
  • Updates!
Curriculum vitae
Google Scholar page

RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS


Connecting with Other People
How can we create better social interactions and conversations, where people feel seen, heard, and appreciated?
This question drives my research on perspective taking, prosocial behavior, conversation, and inclusion. From these projects, I have learned that words and actions can often have (sometimes surprisingly) large impacts!
Surprisingly Happy to Have Helped:Underestimating Prosociality Creates a Misplaced Barrier to Asking for Help
Xuan Zhao and Nicholas Epley
Psychological Science (2022). [paper] [data and materials]
Performing acts of kindness increases well-being, yet people can be reluctant to ask for help that would enable others’ kindness.  We suggest people may be overly reluctant due to miscalibrated expectations about others’ prosocial motivation, underestimating how positively others will feel when asked for help.  A pretest identified that interest in asking for help was correlated with expectations of how helpers would feel, but a series of scenarios, recalled experiences, and live interactions among people in the U.S. (n = 2118) indicated that those needing help consistently underestimated others’ willingness to help, underestimated how positively helpers would feel, and overestimated how inconvenienced helpers would feel.  These miscalibrated expectations stemmed from underestimating helpers’ prosocial motivation, while overestimating compliance motivation.  This research highlights a limitation of construing help-seeking through a lens of compliance by scholars and laypeople alike.  Undervaluing prosociality could create a misplaced barrier to asking for help when needed. [paper] [data and materials on OSF]

Selected media coverage on this line of research:
  • ​Stanford Report (a featured Q&A; also my personal favorite): Asking for help is hard, but people want to help more than we realize, Stanford scholar says
  • New York Times (also a great feature article, too): Go ahead, ask for help. People are happy to give it
  • Adam Grant's twitter post (thank you, Adam!)
Picture
Undersociality: Miscalibrated social cognition can inhibit social connection
Nicholas Epley, Michael Kardas, Xuan Zhao, Stav Atir, and Juliana Schroeder
Trends in Cognitive Sciences. (2022). [paper]
Belonging is a basic need satisfied by signals of warmth and appreciation. Compliments can satisfy others’ need to belong, but recent research suggests that people may underestimate their positive impact on recipients, creating a barrier to giving them more often. Here we assess how people expect compliment recipients to react to receiving multiple compliments over time, compared to the actual experience of recipients. Although people generally expect recipients to adapt to multiple compliments, with each compliment feeling a little less positive and sincere (Experiment 1), an experiment (Experiment 2) in which one person from an acquainted pair received one new compliment for five consecutive days found no evidence of adaptation. Expressers in this experiment also underestimated how positive their recipients would feel overall. An additional experiment (Experiment 3) examining only peoples’ expectations found that people expected less adaptation among recipients when they saw the actual compliments shared in Experiment 2, suggesting that mistaken beliefs about adaptation may stem from an abstract sense that multiple compliments are more similar to each other than they actually are. Belonging is a need that can be satisfied by repeated signs of warmth and appreciation. Underestimating their power may lead people to refrain from expressing these signs more often in daily life.​ [paper]
Insufficiently Complimentary?: Underestimating the Positive Impact of Compliments Creates a Barrier to Expressing Them
Xuan Zhao and Nicholas Epley
​​Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. (2021). [paper] [data and materials]
Compliments increase the well-being of both expressers and recipients, yet people report in a series of surveys giving fewer compliments than they should give, or would like to give. Nine experiments suggest that a reluctance to express genuine compliments partly stems from underestimating the positive impact that compliments will have on recipients.  Participants wrote genuine compliments and then predicted how happy and awkward those compliments would make recipients feel.  Expressers consistently underestimated how positive the recipients would feel while overestimating how awkward recipients would feel (Experiments 1-3, S4).  These miscalibrated expectations are driven partly by perspective gaps in which expressers underestimate how competent—and to a lesser extent how warm—their compliments will be perceived by recipients (Experiments 1-3).  Because people’s interest in expressing a compliment is partly driven by their expectations of the recipient’s reaction, undervaluing a compliment creates a barrier to expressing them (Supplemental Experiments S2, S3, S4).  As a result, directing people to focus on the warmth conveyed by their compliment (Experiment 4), increased interest in expressing it.  We believe these findings reflect a more general tendency to underestimate the positive impact of their prosocial actions on others, leading people to be less prosocial than would be optimal for both their own and others’ well-being. [paper] [Data and materials on OSF]

​Related publication in Harvard Business Review: ​
A Simple Compliment Can Make a Big Difference (with Erica Boothby & Vanessa Bohns)

Selected media coverage on this line of research:
BBC: Why we don't dole out many compliments – but should​
Picture
​Kind Words Do Not Become Tired Words: Undervaluing the Positive Impact of Frequent Compliments
Xuan Zhao and Nicholas Epley
Self & Identity. (2021). [paper] [data and materials]​
Belonging is a basic need satisfied by signals of warmth and appreciation. Compliments can satisfy others’ need to belong, but recent research suggests that people may underestimate their positive impact on recipients, creating a barrier to giving them more often. Here we assess how people expect compliment recipients to react to receiving multiple compliments over time, compared to the actual experience of recipients. Although people generally expect recipients to adapt to multiple compliments, with each compliment feeling a little less positive and sincere (Experiment 1), an experiment (Experiment 2) in which one person from an acquainted pair received one new compliment for five consecutive days found no evidence of adaptation. Expressers in this experiment also underestimated how positive their recipients would feel overall. An additional experiment (Experiment 3) examining only peoples’ expectations found that people expected less adaptation among recipients when they saw the actual compliments shared in Experiment 2, suggesting that mistaken beliefs about adaptation may stem from an abstract sense that multiple compliments are more similar to each other than they actually are. Belonging is a need that can be satisfied by repeated signs of warmth and appreciation. Underestimating their power may lead people to refrain from expressing these signs more often in daily life.​ [paper] [data and materials on OSF]
Picture
Leaving a Choice for Others: Children’s Evaluations of Consider​ate, Socially-Mindful Actions
Xin Zhao, Xuan Zhao, Hyowon Gweon, and Tamar Kushnir
Child Development. (2021). [paper] [data and materials]
​Humans live in an interdependent world where even actions that are primarily self-serving (i.e., intended to fulfill one’s own needs) can have direct or indirect consequences for others. Thus, it seems critical that one be able to read these nuanced social signals and evaluate actions that are primarily self-serving based on the consequences those actions have for others. Over three studies (N = 566 children between ages 4 and 6 and N = 222 adults, from the U.S. and China), we investigated the mentalistic nature, developmental origins, and cultural dependency of such evaluations. We found that, by age 6 but not younger, both U.S. and Chinese children positively evaluate someone who takes something for themselves (a self-serving action) in a way that leaves a choice for another agent over someone who leaves no choice. We also found that these evaluations reflect a genuine understanding of the agent’s considerate intention, rather than a mere preference for item diversity. Furthermore, in light of the similar developmental patterns across cultures, we conclude that evaluations for others’ considerateness in self-serving actions may rely on the critical development of social-cognitive capacities between 4 and 6 years old independent from cultural influences. [paper] [data and materials on OSF]
Picture
Easing into Another Mind: Goal Inference Facilitates Perspective Taking
Xuan Zhao, Corey Cusimano, and Bertram F. Malle
​​Working paper. [abstract]
​
* A portion of this manuscript was published at CogSci '15. [paper]
Mental state inference is a ubiquitous but challenging component of social interaction. In this paper, we propose a facilitating relationship among mental state inferences: Engaging in an initial, easier mental inference makes people more likely to engage in a more difficult one. Drawing on previous evidence, we tested the possibility of a facilitating relationship between two mental state inferences that are known to vary in difficulty: inferring another person’s goals and inferring that person’s unique visual experiences (i.e., “Level-2 perspective taking”). Five studies provided evidence for the hypothesized facilitating relationship: Goal inference increased people’s likelihood of adopting the actors’ perspectives regardless of task complexity, time pressure, and presentation modality. This facilitating relationship suggests new venues for investigating the causal relationship among mental state inferences.

​Related publication:
  • Zhao, X., & Malle, B. F. (2022). Spontaneous perspective taking toward robots: The unique impact of humanlike appearance. Cognition. [paper]
  • ​Zhao, X., Cusimano, C., & Malle, B.F. (2015). In search of triggering conditions for spontaneous visual perspective taking. In Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 2811-2816. [paper]
Picture
Is It a Nine, or a Six? Prosocial and Selective Perspective Taking in Four-Year-Olds
Xuan Zhao, Bertram F. Mall, and Hyowon Gweon
​​Cog Sci '16. [paper]
To successfully navigate the complex social world, people often need to solve the problem of perspective selection: Between two conflicting viewpoints of the self and the other, whose perspective should one take? In two experiments, we show that four-year-olds use others’ knowledge and goals to decide when to engage in visual perspective taking. Children were more likely to take a social partner’s perspective to describe an ambiguous symbol when she did not know numbers and wanted to learn than when she knew numbers and wanted to teach. These results were shown in children’s own responses (Experiment 1) and in their evaluations of others’ responses (Experiment 2). By preschool years, children understand when perspective taking is appropriate and necessary and selectively take others’ perspectives in social interactions. These results provide novel insights into the nature and the development of perspective taking. [paper]
Picture
“Thank You, Because…”: Discussing Disagreement While Finding Common Ground
Xuan Zhao, Heather Caruso, and Jane Risen
Manuscript in preparation. [abstract]
For individuals in diverse communities, engaging one another in open conversation can sometimes be quite difficult. Intending to promote harmony, many are simply taught to avoid initiating or pursuing discussion of differing viewpoints altogether. When such discussions arise, people tend to negate one other’s viewpoints in advocating for their own, creating a combative atmosphere where people feel misunderstood and undervalued. Seeking a conversational technique that would allow a more inclusive dialogue about differences to arise, we developed a novel procedure called “Thank You Because” (TYB). Inspired by the collaborative spirit in improvisational theater, TYB encourages people who have different perspectives to engage gratefully—by identifying and acknowledging value of dialogue. We tested the impact of TYB in lab and field settings, where pairs of strangers engaged in face-to-face conversations about various interpersonal differences (e.g., in personal preferences, or in support for public policies). Compared to a “No, Because” technique, which encouraged the common conversational instinct of poking holes in one another’s arguments, participants using the “Thank You, Because” technique engaged in more inclusive conversations, felt more heard and valued, and perceived more common ground (Studies 1 & 2). Furthermore, compared to a “I Hear That…” technique (Study 2), where participants aimed to show their partner that they understood their viewpoint accurately, the “Thank You, Because” technique showed unique advantages in eliciting the perception of common ground. [short conference paper][participant debriefing]​
Picture
Large-Scale Inclusion Training for Online Community Moderators
Xuan Zhao, MarYam Hamedani, Cinoo Lee, Hazel Markus, and Jennifer Eberhardt
Manuscript in preparation. [abstract]
Technology-mediated discourse communities (e.g., online forums, interest groups, location-based social networks) are becoming increasingly common in people’s daily lives and serve important functions as part of the social fabric. While most conversations are civil and constructive, biased and offensive comments with respect to race and politics will unavoidably show up during a fraught and divisive time. Even though many online communities have volunteer moderators—usually early members of a group—who have the administrative power to moderate inappropriate posts and comments, without effective training, their ability to recognize and counter problematic content can vary widely and may even reflect entrenched biases.
Partnered with a neighborhood-based social network service with a large user base in the U.S., we (SPARQ) conducted a large-scale, pre-registered field experiment featuring a one-hour volunteer inclusion training that is intended to help moderators better identify and counter racially biased language in online conversations. Specifically, we examine two questions in this study: 1) What messaging strategies can motivate more moderators to sign up for and engage in the voluntary training program? 2) To what extent does the course improve moderators’ ability to recognize and address racially biased and unwelcoming content on the platform?
To examine the first question, we randomly assigned 297,322 moderators to receive one of the four short recruitment messages crafted based on prior literature. Across all engagement behaviors, we found that the message focusing on promoting civil and respectful conversations resulted in the highest engagement (email open rate: 29.20%; sign-up rate: 2.77%; completion rate: 0.66%), whereas messages that are framed as either benefiting the community or the moderator themselves were less effective by comparison (email open rates: 24.06% and 23.36%; sign-up rate: 1.93% and 2.03%; completion rate: 0.50% and 0.54%). Finally, the industry-default bias training language ranked in the middle (email open rate: 28.26%; sign-up rate: 2.21%; completion rate: 0.58%).
To address the second question, we first focused on a subset of moderators who completed our survey before (n = 6,106) and after the course (n = 1,873). Comparing their responses in both surveys revealed  significant increases in i) moderators’ self-reported awareness and understanding about racial bias and inclusion, ii) their sensitivity to different types of biased posts that frequently show up on the platform (e.g., posts that claim color-blindness), and iii) behavioral intentions in taking actions against such posts. Beyond positive evidence from survey measures, we are currently tracking and analyzing moderators’ actual behaviors on the platform to examine whether they have played a more active role moderating biased and unwelcoming content on the platform after the training than before, and whether their behavioral changes have lasted for a substantial period of time (6 months) after participating in the intervention.
Finally, our results also highlight at least two obstacles in offering such voluntary inclusion training: motivating users to participate in and completing the training and helping them translate what they learn in the training into concrete actions to reshape online conversation dynamics. Therefore, we are currently exploring other intervention ideas that may encourage more people to sign up for this training and to create a broader impact across their online communities
Picture
Predicting People’s Perceptions of Organizational Statements Following George Floyd’s Death
Xuan Zhao, Rachel Song, Amrita Maitreyi, Clarissa Gutierrez, MarYam Hamedani, Hazel Markus, and Jennifer Eberhardt
Manuscript in preparation. [abstract]
Following George Floyd’s death, many organizations in the U.S. issued statements in support of racial justice. We systematically examined these solidarity statements and people’s reactions to them. In Study 1, we collected and coded 518 statements released by top companies and universities along 144 features. We found that universities are more likely to mention systemic racism and express negative sentiment, while companies are more likely to pledge to donate. However, relatively few statements acknowledged past complicity in racism or pledged to diversify employees. In Study 2, we found that readers’ race, gender, and political orientation influenced their personal awareness of racism, which strongly predicts their expectation for organizations to make public statements, how positively they view a statement and a company, and their intention to engage in anti-racism actions themselves. In addition, statement features impacted those reactions—e.g., those mentioning more negative sentiment and systemic racism were seen as more genuine. In sum, our research highlights how individuals and organizations can interact in a moment of social and cultural change toward racial justice.
Picture

​Connecting with and through Humanlike Machines
​
Machines are entering homes, schools, and offices at a rapid pace, and many seem intriguingly humanlike. How do we perceive and interact with machines that look/act/think like humans? What can our interactions with machines teach us about being human? ​This line of research explores the potentials and perils of making machines humanlike.
Spontaneous Perspective Taking Toward Robots: The Unique Impact of Humanlike Appearance
​Xuan Zhao and Bertram F. Malle
Cognition (2022). [paper] [data and materials]
* A portion of this manuscript was published at 
HRI '16. However, we have revised our stance since then.​
As robots rapidly enter society, how does human social cognition respond to their novel presence? Focusing on one foundational social-cognitive capacity—visual perspective taking—six studies reveal that people spontaneously adopt a robot’s unique perspective and do so with patterns of variation that mirror perspective taking toward humans. As with human agents, visual perspective taking of robots is enhanced when they display goal-directed actions (gaze and reaching vs. mere presence) and when the actions dynamically unfold over time (video vs. photograph). Importantly, perspective taking increases when the robot looks strikingly humanlike (an android) but is absent when the robot looks machine-like. This appearance-driven perspective taking is not due to inferences about the agent’s mind, because it persists when the agent obviously lacks a mind (e.g., a mannequin). Thus, the sight of robots’ superficial human resemblance may trigger and modulate social-cognitive responses in human observers originally evolved for human interaction. [paper] [data and materials]

Related publication:

Zhao, X., Cusimano, C., & Malle, B. F. (2016). Do people spontaneously take a robot’s visual perspective?  In Proceedings of the Eleventh Annual ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction, 335-342. [paper]

​* The most recent manuscript, "Spontaneous Perspective Taking Toward Robots," reflects our most updated findings and views about human perspective taking toward robots. As a result, please read and cite the newest paper instead of the conference proceeding paper in 2016.
A Primer for Conducting Experiments in Human–Robot Interaction
Guy Hoffman and Xuan Zhao
ACM Transactions on Human-Robot Interaction. (2020). [paper]
* Featured as a lead article.
​We provide guidelines for planning, executing, analyzing, and reporting hypothesis-driven experiments in Human–Robot Interaction (HRI). The intended audience are researchers in the eld of HRI who are not trained in empirical research but who are interested in conducting rigorous human-participant studies to support their research. Following the chronological order of research activities and grounded in updated research practices in psychological and behavioral sciences, this primer covers recommended methods and common pitfalls for defining research questions, identifying constructs and hypotheses, choosing appropriate study designs, operationalizing constructs as variables, planning and executing studies, sampling, choosing statistical tools for data analysis, and reporting results. [paper]
What is Human-Like?: Decomposing Robot Human-Like Appearance Using the Anthropomorphic roBOT (ABOT) Database
Elizabeth Phillips, Xuan Zhao, Daniel Ullman, and Bertram F. Malle
​​Proceedings of the Eleventh Annual ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI'18)
* Nominated for Best Paper Award in Theory and Methods in HRI [paper]
Anthropomorphic robots, or robots with human-like appearance features such as eyes, hands, or faces, have drawn considerable attention in recent years. To date, what makes a robot appear human-like has been driven by designers’ and researchers’ intuitions, because a systematic understanding of the range, variety, and relationships among constituent features of anthropomorphic robots is lacking. To fill this gap, we introduce the ABOT (Anthropomorphic roBOT) Database—a collection of 200 images of real- world robots with one or more human-like appearance features (http://www.abotdatabase.info). Harnessing this database, Study 1 uncovered four distinct appearance dimensions (i.e., bundles of features) that characterize a wide spectrum of anthropomorphic robots and Study 2 identified the dimensions and specific features that were most predictive of robots’ perceived human-likeness. With data from both studies, we then created an online estimation tool to help researchers predict how human-like a new robot will be perceived given the presence of various appearance features. The present research sheds new light on what makes a robot look human, and makes publicly accessible a powerful new tool for future research on robots’ human-likeness. [paper]
From Trolley to Autonomous Vehicle: Perception of Responsibility and Moral Norms in Traffic Accidents with Autonomous Cars
Jamy Li, Xuan Zhao, Mu-Jun Cho, Wendy Ju, Bertram F. Malle
SAE Technical Paper. (2016) [paper]
​Autonomous vehicles represent a new class of transportation that may be qualitatively different from existing cars. Two online experiments assessed lay perceptions of moral norms and responsibility for traffic accidents involving autonomous vehicles. In Experiment 1, 120 US adults read a narrative describing a traffic incident between a pedestrian and a motorist. In different experimental conditions, the pedestrian, the motorist, or both parties were at fault. Participants assigned less responsibility to a self-driving car that was at fault than to a human driver who was at fault. Participants confronted with a self-driving car at fault allocated greater responsibility to the manufacturer and the government than participants who were confronted with a human driver at fault did. In Experiment 2, 120 US adults read a narrative describing a moral dilemma in which a human driver or a self-driving car must decide between either allowing five pedestrians to die or taking action to hit a single pedestrian in order to save the five. The “utilitarian” decision to hit the single pedestrian was considered the moral norm for both a self-driving and a human-driven car. Moreover, participants assigned the obligation of setting moral norms for self-driving cars to ethics researchers and to car manufacturers. This research reveals patterns of public perception of autonomous cars and may aid lawmakers and car manufacturers in designing such cars. [paper]
How People Infer a Humanlike Mind from a Robot Body
Xuan Zhao, Elizabeth Phillips, and Bertram Malle
​​Manuscript under review. [working paper] [ABOT Database]
Robots are entering a wide range of society’s private and public settings, often with a strikingly humanlike appearance and emulating a humanlike mind. But what constitutes humanlikeness—in both body and mind—has been conceptually and methodologically unclear. In three studies based on a collection of 251 real-world robots, we report the first programmatic, bottom-up investigation of what constitutes a robot’s humanlike body, how people reason about robot minds, and critically, how specific dimensions of physical appearance are intricately yet systematically related to specific dimensions of inferred mind. Our results challenge three widespread assumptions about robot humanlikeness. First, we show that humanlike appearance is not a unitary construct; instead, three separate appearance dimensions—Body-Manipulators, Face, and Surface—each consist of a unique constellation of human appearance features and jointly constitute a robot’s humanlike body. Second, we find that the widely adopted two-dimensional structure of mind perception (i.e., agency and experience) does not capture systematic variations of inferences about robot minds; rather, a three-dimensional structure, encompassing mental capacities related to Affect, Social-Moral Cognition, and Reality Interaction, emerges from people’s inferences across a wide range of robots. Third, humanlike appearance does not uniformly lead to a global impression of a humanlike mind; instead, people specifically infer robots’ affective and moral capacities from facial and surface appearance, and their reality interaction capacities from body-manipulator appearance. Our findings reveal how physical appearance gives rise to the impression of a mind, even for a robot. [preprint] [ABOT Database]
​​“Hello! How May I Helo You?”: How (Corrected) Errors Humanize a Communicator
​​Shirly Bluvstein*, Xuan Zhao*, Alexandra Barasch, and Juliana Schroeder​ [*equal authorship]
Manuscript under review
.  [working paper] [data and materials]
Today more than ever before, online text-based interactions (e.g., text messages, emails, social media) have become a primary means of communication. Because written communication lacks human nonverbal cues such as appearance, voice, and identity, consumers may struggle to distinguish whether they are interacting online with a human or a chatbot. The current research investigates how typographical errors (“typos”), a common yet overlooked feature in text communication, can humanize a communicator. Across five experiments (N = 2,515) that used ambiguous conversational counterparts (i.e., customer service agents that might be bots), agents (either chatbots or real humans) who made and subsequently corrected a typo were perceived to be more humanlike than ones who made no typo or did not correct the typo. Participants consequently perceived those agents as warmer and more capable of understanding and helping their issues, were more likely to endorse a reward for the agent, and even perceived the company they represented more favorably. These findings provide novel insights into how conversational features may influence customers’ perception of online agents and the brands that use them. The authors discuss theoretical implications for anthropomorphism and social perception and practical implications for companies wishing to humanize their customer service agents.​ [preprint] [data and materials]
Tugging at the Heartstrings: Feeling Human Heartbeat Promotes Prosocial and Cooperative Behaviors
Xuan Zhao, Malte Jung, Desmond C. Ong, Nina Diepenbrock, Jean Costa, Oriel FeldmanHall, Bertram F. Malle
Manuscript in preparation. ​
​
* Received First Prize in the live grant competition at the Annual Meeting of the Society of Personality and Social Psychology (SPSP'17).​
Watch my 2-minute pitch for the SPSP annual grant competition on how feeling another other person's heartbeat increases prosocial behavior (which led to a shank-tank-style live "interrogation" on the main stage at the 2017 SPSP annual convention and, eventually, the first prize).

MEDIA


“Every intellectual has a very special responsibility. He has the privilege and the opportunity of studying. In return, he owes it to his fellow men (or ‘to society’) to represent the results of his study as simply, clearly and modestly as he can.” —Carl Popper

​
​Do people spontaneously take a robot's perspective? Find out in my 6-minute talk at the "Research Matters!" event at Brown University.


​
In this video interview with Anita Nowak, Ph.D, an empathy expert who runs Purposeful Empathy on Youtube, I discussed some of my recent research findings and their implications in everyday life.

COLLABORATORS


I enjoy collaborating with and learning from people from diverse backgrounds. Below are people that I have written papers with (or who I currently owe a paper to).
​
Bertram Malle ​(Brown University,
Department of Cognitive, Linguistic & Psychological Sciences)  ​
Nicholas Epley (University of Chicago, Booth School of Business)  
Jane Risen ​(University of Chicago, Booth School of Business)
​
Hyowon Gweon ​(Stanford University, Department of Psychology) ​
Hazel Markus (Stanford University, Department of Psychology)
Jennifer Eberhardt (Stanford University, Department of Psychology)
MarYam Hamedani (Stanford University, SPARQ, Department of Psychology)

Malte Jung (Cornell University, Department of Information Science)​
Juliana Schroeder (University of California Berkeley, Hass School of Business)

Alixandra Barasch (New York University, Stern School of Business)
Guy Hoffman (Cornell University, Sibley School of Mechanical and Aerospace Engineering)
​Heather Caruso (University of California Los Angeles, Anderson School of Management)
​Elizabeth Phillips (U.S. Air Force of Academy, Department of Behavioral Sciences and Leadership)
​Oriel FeldmanHall (Brown University, Department of Cognitive, Linguistic & Psychological Sciences)
Desmond Ong (
National University of Singapore, Department of Information Systems and Analytics School of Computing)
Tamar Kushnir (Cornell University, Department of Human Development)
Alice (Xin) Zhao ​(Eastern China Normal University, Department of Educational Psychology)
Jamy Li ​(University of Twente, Department of Human-Media Interaction)
Wendy Ju ​(Cornell University, 
Jacobs Technion-Cornell Institute)
Roseanna Sommers (University of Chicago, Law School)
​​Corey Cusimano (Princeton University, Department of Psychology)
Jean Costa ​(Cornell University, Department of Information Science)
Shirley Bluvstein (New York University, Stern School of Business)
Powered by Create your own unique website with customizable templates.
  • Home
  • Research
  • Flourish
  • Teaching
  • Resources
  • Updates!